False recognition production indexes in forward associative strength (FAS) lists with three critical words

  1. Beato Gutiérrez, María Soledad
  2. Arndt, Jason
Journal:
Psicothema

ISSN: 0214-9915

Year of publication: 2014

Volume: 26

Issue: 4

Pages: 457-463

Type: Article

More publications in: Psicothema

Abstract

Background: False memory illusions have been widely studied using the Deese/Roediger-McDermott paradigm (DRM). In this paradigm, participants study words semantically related to a single nonpresented critical word. In a memory test critical words are often falsely recalled and recognized. Method: The present study was conducted to measure the levels of false recognition for seventy-five Spanish DRM word lists that have multiple critical words per list. Lists included three critical words (e.g., HELL, LUCEFER, and SATAN) simultaneously associated with six studied words (e.g., devil, demon, fire, red, bad, and evil). Different levels of forward associative strength (FAS) between the critical words and their studied associates were used in the construction of the lists. Specifically, we selected lists with the highest FAS values possible and FAS was continuously decreased in order to obtain the 75 lists. Results: Six words per list, simultaneously associated with three critical words, were sufficient to produce false recognition. Furthermore, there was wide variability in rates of false recognition (e.g., 53% for DUNGEON, PRISON, and GRATES; 1% for BRACKETS, GARMENT, and CLOTHING). Finally, there was no correlation between false recognition and associative strength. Conclusions: False recognition variability could not be attributed to differences in the forward associative strength.

Bibliographic References

  • Alonso, M.A., Fernández, A., Díez, E., & Beato, M.S. (2004). Índices de producción de falso recuerdo y falso reconocimiento para 55 listas de palabras en castellano [False recall and false recognition production indexes for 55 word-lists in Spanish]. Psicothema, 16, 357-362.
  • Anastasi, J.S., De Leon, A., & Rhodes, M.G. (2005). Normative data for semantically associated Spanish word lists that create false memories. Behavior Research Methods, 37, 631-637.
  • Arndt, J. (2012a). False recollection: Empirical findings and their theoretical implications. In B.H. Ross (Ed.), Psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 81-124). New York: Academic Press.
  • Arndt, J. (2012b). The influence of forward and backward associative strength on false recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 747-756.
  • Beato, M.S., Cadavid, S., Pulido, R.F., & Pinho, M.S. (2013). No effect of stress on false recognition. Psicothema, 25, 25-30.
  • Beato, M.S., & Díez, E. (2011). False recognition production indexes in Spanish for 60 DRM lists with three critical words. Behavior Research Methods, 43, 499-507.
  • Boldini, A., Beato, M.S., & Cadavid, S. (2013). Modality-match effect in false recognition: An event-related potential study. Neuroreport, 24, 108-113.
  • Brainerd, C.J., & Wright, R. (2005). Forward association, backward association, and the false-memory illusion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 554-567.
  • Cabeza, R., Rao, S.M., Wagner, A.D., Mayer, A.R., & Schacter, D.L. (2001). Can medial temporal lobe regions distinguish true from false? An event-related functional MRI study of veridical and illusory recognition memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 4805-4810.
  • Carneiro, P., Fernández, A., Díez, E., García-Marques, L., Ramos, T., & Ferreira, M. (2012). “Identify-to-reject”: A specific strategy to avoid false memories in the DRM paradigm. Memory & Cognition, 40, 252-265.
  • Curran, T., Schacter, D.L., Johnson, M.K., & Spinks, R. (2001). Brain potentials reflect behavioral differences in true and false recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 201-216.
  • Fernández, A., Díez, E., & Alonso, M.A. (2009, July 15). Normas de Asociación libre en castellano, online database [Free Association norms in Spanish]. Retrieved from http://www.usal.es/gimc.
  • Gallo, D.A. (2006). Associative illusions of memory. False memory research in DRM and related tasks. New York: Psychology Press.
  • Gallo, D.A. (2010). False memories and fantastic beliefs: 15 years of the DRM illusion. Memory & Cognition, 38, 833-848.
  • Gallo, D.A., & Roediger, H.L. (2002). Variability among word lists in eliciting memory illusions: Evidence for associative activation and monitoring. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 469-497.
  • Graham, L.M. (2007). Need for cognition and false memory in the Deese-Roediger-McDermott paradigm. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 409-418.
  • Luck, S.J. (2005). An introduction to the event-related potential technique. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • McEvoy, C.L., Nelson, D.L., & Komatsu, T. (1999). What is the connection between true and false memories? The differential roles of interitem associations in recall and recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 1177-1194.
  • Jou, J., & Flores, S. (2013). How are false memories distinguishable from true memories in the Deese-Roediger-McDermott paradigm? A review of the findings. Psychological Research, 77, 671-686.
  • Miller, A.R., Baratta, Ch., Wynween, Ch., & Rosenfeld, J.P. (2001). P300 latency, but not amplitude or topography, distinguishes between true and false recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 27, 354-361.
  • Nelson, D.L., Dyrdal, G.M., & Goodmon, L.B. (2005). What is preexisting strength? Predicting free association probabilities, similarity ratings, and cued recall probabilities. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 711-719.
  • Knott, L.M., Dewhurst, S.A., & Howe, M.L. (2012). What factors underlie associative and categorical memory illusions? The roles of backward associative strength and interitem connectivity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 38, 229-239.
  • Otgaar, H., Peters, M., & Howe, M.L. (2012). Dividing attention lowers children’s but increases adults’ false memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 38, 204-210.
  • Pimentel, E., & Albuquerque, P.B. (2013). Effect of divided attention on the production of false memories in the DRM paradigm: A study of dichotic listening and shadowing. Psicológica, 34, 285-298.
  • Robinson, K.J., & Roediger, H.L.. (1997). Associative processes in false recall and false recognition. Psychological Science, 8, 231-237.
  • Roediger, H.L., III, Watson, J.M., McDermott, K.B., & Gallo, D.A. (2001). Factors that determine false recall: A multiple regression analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 385-407.
  • Stadler, M.A., Roediger, H.L., & McDermott, K.B. (1999). Norms for word lists that create false memories. Memory & Cognition, 27, 494-500.
  • Wiese, H., & Daum, I. (2006). Frontal positivity discriminates true from false recognition. Brain Research, 1075, 183-192.