False recognition in DRM lists with low associationA normative study

  1. Sara Cadavid
  2. María Soledad Beato
Revista:
Psicológica: Revista de metodología y psicología experimental

ISSN: 1576-8597

Año de publicación: 2017

Volumen: 38

Número: 1

Páginas: 133-147

Tipo: Artículo

Otras publicaciones en: Psicológica: Revista de metodología y psicología experimental

Resumen

A wide array of studies have explored memory distortions with the Deese/Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm, where participants study lists of words (e.g., door, glass, pane, shade, ledge, etc.) that are associated to another nonpresented critical word (e.g., WINDOW). On a subsequent memory test, the critical word is often falsely recalled and recognized, even though the critical word was not studied. The present normative study provided false recognition indexes for 48 DRM lists in Spanish with three critical words per list. Lists were constructed with low levels of backward associative strength (BAS), never examined before. Results showed that, even with low association, DRM lists were able to produce false recognition (M = 34%). Also, and despite the low level of association, results showed that there was a wide variability in false recognition per list (e.g., 10% in List 24: ANIMAL [ANIMAL], GATO [CAT], PERRO [DOG], celo [heat], cola [tail], manso [docile], peludo [furry], zarpa [claw], presa [prey]; 62% in List 05: DOLOR [PAIN], MUERTE [DEATH], TRISTEZA [SADNESS], odio [hatred], hambre [hunger], inanición [starvation], morir [to die], huérfano [orphan], consolado [consoled]), replicating previous findings. These new DRM lists will allow researchers to explore false memory effects when words are weakly associated among them.

Información de financiación

This study was partially conducted at Psychology Research Centre (UID/PSI/01662/2013), University of Minho, and supported by the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology and the Portuguese Ministry of Science, Technology and Higher Education through national funds and co-financed by FEDER through COMPETE2020 under the PT2020 Partnership Agreement (POCI-01-0145-FEDER-007653). Also, this work was partially conducted at the Faculty of Psychology, University of Salamanca (Spain) (FPI-USAL-10). The authors report no financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

Financiadores

    • FPI-USAL-10

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Albuquerque, P. B. (2005). Produção de evocações e reconhecimentos falsos em 100 listas de palavras associadas portuguesas [False recall and recognition production in 100 Portuguese associate word list]. Laboratório de Psicologia, 3, 3–12. http://doi.org/10.14417/lp.766.
  • Alonso, M. A., Fernandez, A., Díez, E., & Beato, M. S. (2004). Índices de producción de falso recuerdo y falso reconocimiento para 55 listas de palabras en castellano [False recall and recognition indexes for 55 lists of words in Spanish]. Psicothema, 16, 357–362.
  • Arndt, J. (2012). The influence of forward and backward associative strength on false recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 747–756. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0026375.
  • Arndt, J. (2015). The influence of forward and backward associative strength on false memories for encoding context. Memory, 23, 1093–1111. http://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2014.959527
  • Arndt, J., & Hirshman, E. (1998). True and false recognition in MINERVA2: Explanations from a global matching perspective. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 371–391. http://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1998.2581.
  • Beato, M. S., & Arndt, J. (2014). False recognition production indexes in forward associative strength (FAS) lists with three critical words. Psicothema, 26, 457–463. http://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2014.79.
  • Beato, M. S., Boldini, A., & Cadavid, S. (2012). False memory and level of processing effect: an event-related potential study. Neuroreport, 23, 804–808. http://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e32835734
  • de Beato, M. S., & Díez, E. (2011). False recognition production indexes in Spanish for 60 DRM lists with three critical words. Behavior Research Methods, 43, 499–507. http://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-010-0045-9.
  • Brainerd, C. J., Yang, Y., Reyna, V. F., Howe, M. L., & Mills, B. A. (2008). Semantic processing in “associative” false memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 1035–1053. http://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.15.6.1035.
  • Cadavid, S., & Beato, M. S. (2016). Memory distortion and its avoidance: An event-related potentials study on false recognition and correct rejection. PloS One, 11(10), e0164024. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0164024.
  • Cadavid, S., Beato, M. S., & Fernandez, A. (2012). Falso reconocimiento en listas DRM con tres palabras críticas: Asociación directa vs. inversa [False recognition in DRM lists with three critical words: Forward vs. Backward association]. Psicologica, 33, 39–58.
  • Carneiro, P., Ramos, T., Costa, R. S., Garcia-Marques, L., & Albuquerque, P. B. (2011). Identificabilidade dos temas de listas formadas por associação retrógrada (backward): Contributo para o estudo das memórias falsas [Theme identifiability of backward associative lists: Contribution to false memory research]. Laboratório de Psicologia, 9, 23–34. http://doi.org/10.14417/lp.634.
  • Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 17–22. http://doi.org/10.1037/h0046671.
  • Fernandez, A., Díez, E., & Alonso, M. A. (2011). Materiales normativos en castellano: Normas de asociación libre [Normative materials in Castilian: Free association norms]. Retrieved March 15, 2011, from http://www.usal.es/gimc/nalc
  • Gallo, D. A. (2010). False memories and fantastic beliefs: 15 years of the DRM illusion. Memory & Cognition, 38, 833–848. http://doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.7.833.
  • Gallo, D. A., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2002). Variability among word lists in eliciting memory illusions: Evidence for associative activation and monitoring. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 469–497. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00013-X
  • Howe, M. L., Gagnon, N., & Thouas, L. (2008). Development of false memories in bilingual children and adults. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 669–681. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.09.001.
  • Knott, L. M., Dewhurst, S. A., & Howe, M. L. (2012). What factors underlie associative and categorical memory illusions? The roles of backward associative strength and interitem connectivity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 229–239. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0025201
  • McEvoy, C. L., Nelson, D. L., & Komatsu, T. (1999). What is the connection between true and false memories? The differential roles of interitem associations in recall and recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 1177–1194. http://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.25.5.1177.
  • Robinson, K. J., & Roediger, H. L. III. (1997). Associative processes in false recall and false recognition. Psychological Science, 8, 231–237. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00417.x
  • Roediger, H. L. III, & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803–814. http://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.803.
  • Roediger, H. L. III., Watson, J. M., McDermott, K. B., & Gallo, D. A. (2001). Factors that determine false recall: A multiple regression analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 385–407. http://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196177.
  • Stadler, M. A., Roediger, H. L. III., & McDermott, K. B. (1999). Norms for word lists that create false memories. Memory & Cognition, 27, 494–500. http://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211543