Comunicación no verbal y toma de decisiones en el ámbito judicial

  1. Fernando Gordillo León 1
  2. Rafael Manuel López Pérez 2
  3. Lilia Mestas Hernández 3
  4. Miguel Ángel Pérez Nieto 4
  5. José M. Arana Martínez 5
  1. 1 Universidad Camilo José Cela
    info

    Universidad Camilo José Cela

    Villanueva de la Cañada, España

    ROR https://ror.org/03f6h9044

  2. 2 Fundación Universitaria Behavior & Law
  3. 3 Facultad de Estudios Superiores Zaragoza
  4. 4 Universidda Camilo José Cela
  5. 5 Universidad de Salamanca
    info

    Universidad de Salamanca

    Salamanca, España

    ROR https://ror.org/02f40zc51

Revue:
Behavior & Law Journal

ISSN: 2444-4170

Année de publication: 2016

Volumen: 2

Número: 1

Pages: 51-58

Type: Article

DOI: 10.47442/BLJ.V2.I1.28 DIALNET GOOGLE SCHOLAR lock_openAccès ouvert editor

D'autres publications dans: Behavior & Law Journal

Résumé

Introduction. Non-verbal communication (NVC) comprises means of information transmission that are poorly controlled by the transmitter, to the point that it becomes an element of great influence in decision-making in areas as important as the judicial context. The objective of this review article is to analyse recent research into the effects of the NVC of both the accused and the victim on judicial decisions, as well as the importance of the information contexts generated around these forms of communication. Development. The expression of emotions, whether from the accused, the victim, the victim’s family or even defence lawyers, generates positive and negative trends that could determine the outcome of the trial to a certain extent. Moreover, the information surrounding the trial, although not always true, could be biasing the information processing and therefore the objectivity of the judicial agents involved. The consistency between expected and perceived information, in particular in regard to what one expects to believe given a specific type of crime, is fundamental in understanding these processes. Discussion and conclusions. This review shows the need for understanding to control the different verbal and non-verbal variables involved in the channelling of information in a trial, with a view to avoiding biased decisions that can lead to the wrong verdict.

Références bibliographiques

  • Alexy, R. (1989). Teoría de la argumentación jurídica: la teoría del discurso racional como teoría de la fundamentación jurídica, traducción de Manuel Atienza e Isabel Espejo, Centro de Estudios Constitucionales, Madrid.
  • Ask, K. y Landström, S. (2010). Why emotions matter: Expectancy violation and affective response mediate the emotional victim effect. Law and Human Behavior, 34, 392-401.
  • Barrett, L. F., Lindquist, K. y Gendron, M. (2007). Language as a context for the perception of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 327-332.
  • Barret, L. F., Mesquita, B. y Gendron, M. (2011). Context in emotion perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 286.
  • Bornstein, B. A., Rung, L. M. y Miller, M. K. (2002). The effects of defendant remorse on mock juror decisions in a malpractice case. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 20, 393-409.
  • Brodsky, S. L. (2004). Coping with cross-examination and other pathways to effective testimony. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  • Brodsky, S. L. (2009). Principles and practice of trial consultation. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
  • Carroll, J. M. y Russell, J. A. (1996). Do facial expressions signal specific emotions? Judging emotion from the face in context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 205-218.
  • Castelli, P. y Goodman, G. S. (2014). Children's perceived emotional behavior at disclosure and prosecutors’ evaluations. Child Abuse & Neglect, 38, 1521-1532.
  • Colín, R. y Monterrubio, E. (2010). La subjetivodad como factor determinante en el sentido de la sentencia penal. CIENCIA ergo sum, 17(2), 177-182.
  • Corwin, E. P., Cramer, R. J., Griffin, D. A. y Brodsky, S. L. (2012). Defendant remorse, need for affect, and juror sentencing decision. Journal of de American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 40, 41-49.
  • Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, rationality and the human brain. New York: Putnam (Grosset Books).
  • Denzin, N. K. y Lincoln, Y. S. (2005). The SAGE handbook of qualitative research 3rd ed., Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.
  • Domínguez, M. R. (2008). La importancia de la Comunicación no verbal en el desarrollo cultural de las sociedades. Razón y Palabra, 70.
  • Dunn, J. R. y Schweitzer, M. E. (2005). Feeling and believing: The influence of emotion on trust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 736-748.
  • Ekman, P. (1972). Emotion in the human face (General Psychology). Amsterdam, Elservier.
  • Ekman, P. y Friesen, W. V. (1969). Nonverbal leakage and clues to deception. Psychiatry Journal for the Study of Interpersonal Processes, 32(1), 88-106.
  • Fonseca, Y. M. (2000). Comunicación oral. Fundamentos y práctica estratégica. Pearson Educación, México, 2000.
  • Gold, G. J. y Weiner, B. (2000). Remorse, confession, group identity, and expectancies about repeating a transgression. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 22, 291-300.
  • Gordillo, F. (2015). Los juicios paralelos en el proceso judicial. Actualidad Criminológica, 1, 38-41.
  • Gordillo, F., López, R. M., Mestas, L. y Corbi, B. (2014). Comunicación no verbal en la negociación: La importancia de saber expresar lo que se dice. Revista Electrónica de Psicología Iztacala, 17(2), 646-666.
  • Gordillo, F., Pérez, M.A., Castillo, G., Mestas, L., Arana, J.M. y López, R.M. (2016). Escritos de Psicología (en prensa).
  • Grammer, K., Renninger, L. y Fischer, B. (2004). Disco clothing, female sexual motivation and relationship status; Is She Dressed to Impress? Journal of Sexual Research, 41(1), 66-74.
  • Hackett, L., Day, A. y Mohr, P. (2008). Expectancy violation and perceptions of rape victim credibility. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 13, 323-334.
  • Hargie, O., 2011. Skilled interpersonal communication 5th ed. London: Routledge.
  • Heath, W. P. (2009). Arresting and convicting the innocent: the potential role of an “inappropriate” emotional display in the accused. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 27(3), 313-332.
  • Heerdink, M. W., Van Kleef, G. A., Homan, A. C. y Fischer, A. H. (2015). Emotional expressions as social signals of rejection and acceptance: Evidence from the affect misattribution paradigm. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 56, 60-68.
  • Kleinke, C. L, Wallis, R. y Stalder, K. (1992). Evaluation of a rapist as a function of expressed intent and remorse. Journal of Social Psychology, 132, 525-537.
  • Klippenstein, M. A. y Schuller, R. (2012). Perceptions of sexual assault: Expectancies regarding the emotional response of a rape victim over time. Psychology, Crime and Law, 18, 79-94.
  • Knutson, B. (1996). Facial expressions of emotion influence interpersonal trait inferences. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 20, 165-182.
  • Landström, S., Ask, K., Sommar, C. y Willén, R. (2013). Children's testimony and the emotional victim effect. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 20, 365-383.
  • Lerner, J. S. y Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 146-159.
  • Lerner, J. S., Small, D. A. y Loewenstein, G. (2004). Heart strings and purse strings: Carryover effects of emotions on economic decisions. Psychological Science, 15, 337-341.
  • Lens, K. M. E., Van Doorn, J., Pemberton, A. y Bogaerts, S. (2013). You shouldn't feel that way! Extending the emotional victim effect through the mediating role of expectancy violation. Psychology, Crime & Law, 20, 326-338.
  • LeVan, E. A. (1984). Nonverbal communication in the courtroom: Attorney beware. Law and Psychology Review, 8, 83-104.
  • López, R. M. (2012). Reconocimiento de expresión emocional no verbal por miembros de las fuerzas armadas y cuerpos de seguridad del estado. Tesis Doctoral. Madrid: Universidad Camilo José Cela.
  • López, R., Gordillo, F. y Soto, J. E. (2016). Protocolo NBAM (Nonverbal Behavior Analysis Matrix). In R. López, F. Gordillo y M. Grau (Eds.), Comportamiento no Verbal: más allá de la comunicación y el lenguaje. Madrid: Pirámide.
  • Luengo, J. La educación como hecho. En Teorías e instituciones contemporáneas de educación (ed. Pozo, M., Álvarez, J.L. y Luego, J.). Madrid, Biblioteca Nueva, 2004.
  • Magnussen, S. y Wessel, E. (2010). Displayed emotions in court: Effects on credibility judgements. En P. A. Granhag (Ed.), Forensic psychology in context: Nordic and international perspectives, UK: Willan Publishing.
  • Masuda, T., Ellsworth, P. C., Mesquita, B., Leu, J., Tanida, S. y Van de Veerdonk, E. (2008). Placing the face in context: Cultural differences in the perception of facial emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 365-381.
  • Montalvo, J. A. (2012). Los juicios paralelos en el proceso penal: ¿Anomalía democrática o mal necesario? Universitas. Revista e Filosofía, Derecho y Política, 16, 105-124.
  • Monterrubbianesi, M. G. (2011). La comunicación no verbal del español actual a partir de la filmografía de Pedro Almodovar y su aplicación a la didáctica de E/LE. Tesis doctoral: Universidad de Granada.
  • Muñoz, A. (2011). La influencia de los sesgos cognitivos en las decisiones jurisdiccionales: el factor humano. Una aproximación. InDret Revista para el Análisis del Derecho, 2.
  • Nagle, J. E., Brodsky, S. L. y Weeter, K. (2014). Gender, smiling, and witness credibility in actual trials. Behavioral Science & the Law, 32, 195-206.
  • Paternoster, R. y Deise, J. (2011). A heavy thumb on the scale: The effect of victim impact evidence on capital decision making. Criminology, 49(1), 129-161.
  • Proeve M. J. y Howells, K. (2006). Effects of remorse and shame and criminal justice experience on judgements about a sex offender. Psychology, Crime & Law 12, 145-61.
  • Righart, R. y de Gelder, B. (2006). Context influences early perceptual analysis of faces. An electrophysiological study. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 1249-1257.
  • Russell, J. A. y Fehr, B. (1987). Relativity in the perception of emotion in facial expressions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 116, 223-237.
  • Salekin, R. T., Ogloff, J. R. P., McFarland, C. y Rogers, R. (1995).
  • Influencing jurors’ perceptions of guilt: Expression of emotionality during testimony. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 13, 293-305.
  • Salerno, J. M. (2012). One angry woman: Emotion expression and minority influence in a jury deliberation context (Doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois at Chicago). Retrieved from http://indigo.uic.edu/handle/10027/9602
  • Sánchez, G. (2009). La comunicación no verbal, en Jornadas de formación de profesores e E/LE: estrategias de enseñanza y aprendizaje de español en China, marco E/LE, num.8, pp1-16.
  • Reeve, J. (1994). Motivación y emoción. Madrid, España: Ediciones Mc Graw Hill.
  • Remland, M. S. (1993). The importance of nonverbal communication in the courtroom. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Eastern Communication Association (84th, New Haven, CT, April 29-May 2, 1993).
  • Rose, M. R., Nadler, J. y Clark, J. (2006). Appropriately upset? Emotion norms and perceptions of crime victims. Law and Human Behavior, 30, 203-219.
  • Richerson, P. J. y Boyd, R. (2010). Why possibly language evolved. Biolinguistics, 4, 289-306.
  • Tiedens, L. Z. (2001). Anger and advancement versus sadness and subjugation: The effect of negative emotion expressions on social status conferral. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 86-94.
  • Van Doorn, E., Van Kleef, G. y Van der Pligt, J. (2014). How emotional expressions shape prosocial behavior: Interpersonal effects of anger and disappointment on compliance with requests. Motivation and Emotion, 39, 128-141.
  • Wevodau, A. L., Cramer, R. J., Clak III, J. W. y Kehn, A. (2014). The role of emotion and cognition in juror perceptions of victim impact statements. Social Justice Research, 27, 45-66.
  • Wessel, E., Bollingmo, G. C., Sönsteby, C., Nielsen, L. M., Eilertsen, D. E. y Magnussen, S. (2012). The emotional witness effect: Story content, emotional valence and credibility of a male suspect. Psychology, Crime & Law, 18, 417-430.
  • Wessel, E., Magnussen, S. y Melinder, A. M. D. (2013). Expressed emotions and perceived credibility of child mock victims disclosing physical abuse. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 27, 611-616. doi: 10.1002/acp.2935
  • Winkel, F. W. y Koppelaar, L. (1991). Rape victims’ style of self-presentation and secondary victimization by the environment: An experiment. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 6, 29-40.
  • Wredem O. The Role of Emotions in Judgments of Crime Victims. Tesis; University of Gothenburg: 2015.
  • Zhong, R., Baranoski, M., Feigenson, N., Davidson, l., Buchanan, A. y Zonana, H. V. (2014). So You’re Sorry? The Role of Remorse in Criminal Law. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 42, 39-48.