Interpretive diversity understanding, parental practices, and contextual factors involved in primary school-age children’s cheating and lying behavior

  1. Narcisa Prodan 1
  2. Melania Moldovan 1
  3. Simina Alexandra Cacuci 1
  4. Laura Visu Petra 1
  1. 1 Babeș-Bolyai University
    info

    Babeș-Bolyai University

    Cluj, Rumanía

    ROR https://ror.org/02rmd1t30

Revista:
EJIHPE: European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education

ISSN: 2174-8144 2254-9625

Año de publicación: 2022

Volumen: 12

Número: 11

Páginas: 1621-1643

Tipo: Artículo

DOI: 10.3390/EJIHPE12110114 DIALNET GOOGLE SCHOLAR lock_openDialnet editor

Otras publicaciones en: EJIHPE: European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education

Resumen

Dishonesty is an interpersonal process that relies on sophisticated socio-cognitive mechanisms embedded in a complex network of individual and contextual factors. The present study examined parental rearing practices, bilingualism, socioeconomic status, and children’s interpretive diversity understanding (i.e., the ability to understand the constructive nature of the human mind) in relation to their cheating and lie-telling behavior. 196 school-age children (9–11 years old) participated in a novel trivia game-like temptation resistance paradigm to elicit dishonesty and to verify their interpretive diversity understanding. Results revealed that children’s decision to cheat and lie was positively associated with their understanding of the constructive nature of the human mind and with parental rejection. Children with rejective parents were more likely to lie compared to their counterparts. This may suggest that understanding social interactions and the relationship with caregivers can impact children’s cheating behavior and the extent to which they are willing to deceive about it. Understanding the constructive nature of the mind was also a positive predictor of children’s ability to maintain their lies. Finally, being bilingual and having a higher socioeconomic status positively predicted children’s deception, these intriguing results warranting further research into the complex network of deception influences.

Información de financiación

Financiadores

  • Unitatea Executiva Pentru Finantarea Invatamantului Superior a Cercetarii Dezvoltarii si Inovarii
    • PN-III-P1-1.1-TE-2019-1075
    • PN-III-P1-1.1-TE-2021-1051

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Achenbach, T.M.; Edelbrock, C.S. Behavioral problems and competencies reported by parents of normal and disturbed children aged four through sixteen. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 1981, 46, 1–82.
  • Alan, S.; Ertac, S.; Gumren, M. Cheating and incentives in a performance context: Evidence from a field experiment on children. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2020, 179, 681–701.
  • Alloway, T.P.; McCallum, F.; Alloway, R.G.; Hoicka, E. Liar, liar, working memory on fire: Investigating the role of working memory in childhood verbal deception. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2015, 137, 30–38.
  • Andreea, S.M. Rolul educa¸tiei asupra cre¸sterii economice în România. Ecostudent–Rev. De Cercet. ¸Stiin¸tifică A Studen¸tilor Econ. 2013, 1, 45–49.
  • Baker, C. Becoming bilingual through bilingual education. In Handbook of Multilingualism and Multilingual Communication; Auer, P., Wei, L., Eds.; De Gruyter Academic Publishing: Berlin, Germany, 2007; Volume 5, pp. 131–152.
  • Baudat, S.; Van Petegem, S.; Antonietti, J.P.; Zimmermann, G. Parental solicitation and adolescents’ information management: The moderating role of autonomy-supportive parenting. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2020, 29, 426–441.
  • Becker, H.J. Who’s wired and who’s not: Children’s access to and use of computer technology. Future Child. 2000, 10, 44–75.
  • Bialystok, E.; Craik, F.I. How does bilingualism modify cognitive function? Attention to the mechanism. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2022, 29, 1246–1269.
  • Bøe, T.; Sivertsen, B.; Heiervang, E.; Goodman, R.; Lundervold, A.J.; Hysing, M. Socioeconomic status and child mental health: The role of parental emotional well-being and parenting practices. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2014, 42, 705–715.
  • Bornstein, M.H. Parenting in acculturation: Two contemporary research designs and what they tell us. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2017, 15, 195–200.
  • Bureau, J.S.; Mageau, G.A. Parental autonomy support and honesty: The mediating role of identification with the honesty value and perceived costs and benefits of honesty. J. Adolesc. 2014, 37, 225–236.
  • Buschgens, C.J.; Van Aken, M.A.; Swinkels, S.H.; Ormel, J.; Verhulst, F.C.; Buitelaar, J.K. Externalizing behaviors in preadolescents: Familial risk to externalizing behaviors and perceived parenting styles. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2010, 19, 567–575.
  • Caldwell-Harris, C.L.; Ayçiçe ˘gi-Dinn, A. Emotion and lying in a non-native language. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2009, 71, 193–204.
  • Callender, K.A.; Olson, S.L.; Kerr, D.C.; Sameroff, A.J. Assessment of cheating behavior in young school-age children: Distinguishing normative behaviors from risk markers of externalizing psychopathology. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2010, 39, 776–788.
  • Carl, T.; Bussey, K. Contextual and age-related determinants of children’s lie telling to conceal a transgression. Infant Child Dev. 2019, 28, e2129.
  • Carpendale, J.I.; Chandler, M.J. On the distinction between false belief understanding and subscribing to an interpretive theory of mind. Child Dev. 1996, 67, 1686–1706.
  • Costa, A.; Hernández, M.; Sebastián-Gallés, N. Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition 2008, 106, 59–86.
  • Cumsille, P.; Darling, N.; Martínez, M.L. Shading the truth: The patterning of adolescents’ decisions to avoid issues, disclose, or lie to parents. J. Adolesc. 2010, 33, 285–296.
  • De Bruin, A.; Dick, A.S.; Carreiras, M. Clear theories are needed to interpret differences: Perspectives on the bilingual advantage debate. Neurobiol. Lang. 2021, 2, 433–451.
  • Ding, X.P.; Omrin, D.S.; Evans, A.D.; Fu, G.; Chen, G.; Lee, K. Elementary school children’s cheating behavior and its cognitive correlates. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2014, 121, 85–95.
  • Duñabeitia, J.A.; Costa, A. Lying in a native and foreign language. Psychon. Bullerin Rev. 2015, 22, 1124–1129.
  • Eguaras, S.G.; Erostarbe, I.I. The role of parenting practices in children’s antisocial lying: A systematic review. Psychol. Pap. 2021, 42, 152–159.
  • Evans, A.D.; Lee, K. Verbal deception from late childhood to middle adolescence and its relation to executive functioning skills. Dev. Psychol. 2011, 47, 1108.
  • Fibla, L.; Kosie, J.E.; Kircher, R.; Lew-Williams, C.; Byers-Heinlein, K. Bilingual language development in infancy: What can we do to support bilingual families? Policy Insights Behav. Brain Sci. 2022, 9, 35–43.
  • Foley, S.; Hughes, C. Family influences on theory of mind in middle childhood. In Theory of Mind in Middle Childhood and Adolescence; Devine, R.T., Lecce, S., Eds.; Routledge: England, UK, 2021; pp. 101–121.
  • Goetz, P.J. The effects of bilingualism on theory of mind development. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2003, 6, 1–15.
  • Grosjean, F. Bilingual. Life and reality. Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2010.
  • Grusec, J.E.; Davidov, M. Integrating different perspectives on socialization theory and research: A domain-specific approach. Child Dev. 2010, 81, 687–709.
  • Harris, P.L.; Núntez, M. Understanding of permission rules by preschool children. Child Dev. 1996, 67, 1572–1591.
  • Hertwig, R.; Mazar, N. Toward a taxonomy and review of honesty interventions. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2022, 47, 101410.
  • Heyman, G.D.; Zhao, L.; Compton, B.J.; Lee, K. Dishonesty in young children. In Dishonesty in Behavioral Economics; Bucciol, A., Montinari, N., Eds.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2019; pp. 17–29.
  • Hoff, E.; Laursen, B. Socioeconomic status and parenting. In Handbook of Parenting: Biology and Ecology of Parenting; Bornstein, M.H., Ed.; Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group: London, UK, 2019; pp. 421–447.
  • Hoff, E.; Laursen, B.; Tardif, T. Socioeconomic status and parenting. In Handbook of Parenting: Biology and Ecology of Parenting; Bornstein, M.H., Ed.; Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2002; Volume 2, pp. 231–252.
  • Hoffman, L.W. Methodological issues in studies of SES, parenting, and child development. In Socioeconomic Status, Parenting, and Child Development; Bornstein, M.H., Bradley, R.H., Eds.; Taylor & Francis Group: New York, NY, USA, 2003; pp. 125–143.
  • Huang, R.; Baker, E.R.; Battista, C.; Liu, Q. Executive Function and Theory of Mind in Children Living in Poverty: A Short-term Longitudinal Study. J. Cogn. Dev. 2022, 23, 751–775.
  • Kelly, J.A.; Worell, L. Personality characteristics, parent behaviors, and sex of subject in relation to cheating. J. Res. Personal. 1978, 12, 179–188.
  • Kotaman, H. Impact of rewarding and parenting styles on young children’s cheating behavior. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2017, 14, 127–140.
  • Kovács, Á.M. Early bilingualism enhances mechanisms of false-belief reasoning. Dev. Sci. 2009, 12, 48–54.
  • Lalonde, C.E.; Chandler, M.J. Children’s understanding of interpretation. New Ideas Psychol. 2002, 20, 163–198.
  • Lee, J.Y.S.; Imuta, K. Lying and Theory of Mind: A Meta-Analysis. Child Dev. 2021, 92, 536–553.
  • Letourneau, N.L.; Duffett-Leger, L.; Levac, L.; Watson, B.; Young-Morris, C. Socioeconomic status and child development: A meta-analysis. J. Emot. Behav. Disord. 2013, 21, 211–224.
  • Lewis, M.; Stanger, C.; Sullivan, M.W. Deception in 3-year-olds. Dev. Psychol. 1989, 25, 439.
  • Lovett, S.B.; Pillow, B.H. Development of the ability to distinguish between comprehension and memory: Evidence from goal-state evaluation tasks. J. Educ. Psychol. 1995, 88, 546.
  • Lynch, A. Bilingualism and second language acquisition. In Second and Foreign Language Education, Encyclopedia of Language and Education; Van Deusen-Scholl, N., May, S., Eds.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2017; Volume 4, pp. 43–55.
  • Ma, F.; Evans, A.D.; Liu, Y.; Luo, X.; Xu, F. To lie or not to lie? The influence of parenting and theory-of-mind understanding on three-year-old children’s honesty. J. Moral Educ. 2015, 44, 198–212.
  • Maftei, A.; Bumbaru, E.V.; Grigore, A.N.; Holman, A.C. Parental rejection and moral disengagement predict Romanian adolescents’ bullying behavior. Int. J. Educ. Psychol. Community 2020, 10, 163–185.
  • Malloy, L.C.; Mugno, A.P.; Waschbusch, D.A.; Pelham, W.E.; Talwar, V. Parents’ attitudes about and socialization of honesty and dishonesty in typically-developing children and children with disruptive behavior disorders. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2019, 47, 299–312.
  • Miller, S.A. Theory of Mind: Beyond the Preschool Years; Psychology Press: England, UK, 2012.
  • Moldovan, M.; Prodan, N.; Coman, A.D.; Visu-Petra, L. Deep dive into the constructive mind: Relating interpretive diversity understanding to anxiety symptoms and parental practices in middle childhood. J. Evid. -Based Psychother. 2022, 22, 55–86.
  • Moldovan, M.; Seucan, D.T.; Visu-Petra, L. Pre-and post-theory of mind and deception: Commentary on Walczyk and Fargerson (2019). New Ideas Psychol. 2020, 56, 100754.
  • National Institute of Statistics. Earnings-Since 1991, the Monthly Series; National Institute of Statistics: Washington, DC, USA, 2021; Available online: https://insse.ro/cms/en/content/earnings-1991-monthly-series (accessed on 13 July 2022).
  • O’Connor, A.M.; Dykstra, V.W.; Evans, A.D. Executive functions and young children’s lie-telling and lie maintenance. Dev. Psychol. 2020, 56, 1278–1289.
  • O’Connor, A.M.; Evans, A.D. The role of theory of mind and social skills in predicting children’s cheating. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2019, 179, 337–347.
  • O’Reilly, J.; Peterson, C.C. Theory of mind at home: Linking authoritative and authoritarian parenting styles to children’s social understanding. Early Child Dev. Care 2014, 184, 1934–1947.
  • Palo¸s, R.; Drobot, L. The impact of family influence on the career choice of adolescents. Procedia-Soc. Behav. Sci. 2010, 2, 3407–3411.
  • Perkins, S.A.; Turiel, E. To lie or not to lie: To whom and under what circumstances. Child Dev. 2007, 78, 609–621.
  • Perris, C.; Jacobsson, L.; Linndström, H.; von Knorring, L.; Perris, H. Development of a new inventory for assessing memories of parental rearing behaviour. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 1980, 61, 265–274.
  • Pillow, B.H.; Mash, C. Children’s understanding of misinterpretation: Source identification and perspective-taking. Merrill-Palmer Q. 1998, 44, 129–140.
  • Polak, A.; Harris, P.L. Deception by young children following noncompliance. Dev. Psychol. 1999, 35, 561.
  • Qualls, R. The relationship between disciplinary practices in childhood and academic dishonesty in college students. Coll. Stud. J. 2014, 48, 362–374.
  • Robila, M. Child development and family functioning within the Romanian context. In Families in Eastern Europe (Contemporary Perspectives in Family Research); Robila, M., Ed.; Elsevier Ltd.: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2004; Volume 5, pp. 141–154.
  • Robila, M.; Krishnakumar, A. The impact of maternal depression and parenting behaviors on adolescents’ psychological functioning in Romania. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2006, 15, 70–81.
  • Ross, H.S.; Recchia, H.E.; Carpendale, J.I. Making sense of divergent interpretations of conflict and developing an interpretive understanding of mind. J. Cogn. Dev. 2005, 6, 571–592.
  • Sai, L.; Shang, S.; Tay, C.; Liu, X.; Sheng, T.; Fu, G.; Lee, K. Theory of mind, executive function, and lying in children: A meta-analysis. Dev. Sci. 2021, 24, e13096.
  • Schroeder, S.R. Do bilinguals have an advantage in theory of mind? A meta-analysis. Front. Commun. 2018, 3, 36.
  • Schwanenflugel, P.J.; Fabricius, W.V.; Alexander, J. Developing theories of mind: Understanding concepts and relations between mental activities. Child Dev. 1994, 65, 1546–1563.
  • Schwanenflugel, P.J.; Fabricius, W.V.; Noyes, C.R. Developing organization of mental verbs: Evidence for the development of a constructivist theory of mind in middle childhood. Cogn. Dev. 1996, 11, 265–294.
  • Seucan, D.T.; Szekely-Copîndean, R.D.; Ding, X.P.; Visu-Petra, L. Give and take: A microgenetic study of preschoolers’ deceptive and prosocial behavior in relation to their socio-cognitive development. Acta Psychol. 2022, 230, 103714.
  • Stouthamer-Loeber, M.; Loeber, R. Boys who lie. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 1986, 14, 551–564.
  • Suchotzki, K.; Gamer, M. The language of lies: Behavioral and autonomic costs of lying in a native compared to a foreign language. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2018, 147, 734–746.
  • Tafreshi, D.; Racine, T.P. Children’s interpretive theory of mind: The role of mothers’ personal epistemologies and mother-child talk about interpretation. Cogn. Dev. 2016, 39, 57–70.
  • Talwar, V.; Lee, K. Development of lying to conceal a transgression: Children’s control of expressive behaviour during verbal deception. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2002, 26, 436–444.
  • Talwar, V.; Crossman, A. From little white lies to filthy liars: The evolution of honesty and deception in young children. Adv. Child Dev. Behav. 2011, 40, 139–179.
  • Talwar, V.; Crossman, A. Liar, liar . . . sometimes: Understanding Social-Environmental Influences on the Development of Lying. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2022, 47, 101374.
  • Talwar, V.; Gordon, H.M.; Lee, K. Lying in the elementary school years: Verbal deception and its relation to second-order belief understanding. Dev. Psychol. 2007, 43, 804–810.
  • Talwar, V.; Lavoie, J.; Crossman, A.M. Carving Pinocchio: Longitudinal examination of children’s lying for different goals. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2019, 181, 34–55.
  • Talwar, V.; Lavoie, J.; Crossman, A.M. Socialization of lying scale: Development and validation of a parent measure of socialization of truth and lie-telling behavior. Appl. Dev. Sci. 2021, 26, 553–566.
  • Talwar, V.; Lavoie, J.; Gomez-Garibello, C.; Crossman, A.M. Influence of social factors on the relation between lie-telling and children’s cognitive abilities. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2017, 159, 185–198.
  • Talwar, V.; Lee, K. Little Liars: Origins of Verbal Deception in Children. In Origins of the Social Mind; Itakura, S., Fujita, K., Eds.; Springer: Tokyo, Japan, 2008; pp. 157–178.
  • Thijssen, S.; Wildeboer, A.; van Ijzendoorn, M.H.; Muetzel, R.L.; Langeslag, S.J.; Jaddoe, V.W.; Verhulst, F.C.; Tiemeier, H.; Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J.; White, T. The honest truth about deception: Demographic, cognitive, and neural correlates of child repeated deceptive behavior. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2017, 162, 225–241.
  • Tong, D.; Talwar, V. Understanding the development of honesty in children through the domains-of-socialization approach. Infant Child Dev. 2021, 30, e2268.
  • Visu-Petra, L.; Prodan, N.; Talwar, V. Children’s Lies: Intersecting Cognitive Development, Theory of Mind, and Socialization. In The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development, 3rd ed.; Smith, P.K., Hart, C.H., Eds.; Routledge: England, UK, 2022; pp. 668–685.
  • Walczyk, J.J.; Fargerson, C. A cognitive framework for understanding development of the ability to deceive. New Ideas Psychol. 2019, 54, 82–92.
  • Waller, R.; Gardner, F.; Hyde, L.W.; Shaw, D.S.; Dishion, T.J.; Wilson, M.N. Do harsh and positive parenting predict parent reports of deceitful-callous behavior in early childhood? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2012, 53, 946–953.
  • Weimer, A.A.; Dowds, S.J.P.; Fabricius, W.V.; Schwanenflugel, P.J.; Suh, G.W. Development of constructivist theory of mind from middle childhood to early adulthood and its relation to social cognition and behavior. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2017, 154, 28–45.
  • Weimer, A.A.; Warnell, K.R.; Ettekal, I.; Cartwright, K.B.; Guajardo, N.R.; Liew, J. Correlates and antecedents of theory of mind development during middle childhood and adolescence: An integrated model. Dev. Rev. 2021, 59, 100945.
  • Wellman, H.M.; Liu, D. Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Dev. 2004, 75, 523–541.
  • Yu, C.L.; Kovelman, I.; Wellman, H.M. How Bilingualism Informs Theory of Mind Development. Child Dev. Perspect. 2021, 15, 154–159.
  • Zhao, L.; Heyman, G.D.; Chen, L.; Lee, K. Praising young children for being smart promotes cheating. Psychol. Sci. 2017, 28, 1868–1870.
  • Zhao, L.; Zheng, Y.; Mao, H.; Zheng, J.; Compton, B.J.; Fu, G.; Lee, K. Using environmental nudges to reduce academic cheating in young children. Dev. Sci. 2021, 24, e13108.